Journal Mobile Options
Table of Contents
Vol. 53, No. 5, 2010
Issue release date: January 2011
Section title: Original Paper
Human Development 2010;53:264–277
(DOI:10.1159/000321289)

Grammatical Difficulties in Children with Specific Language Impairment: Is Learning Deficient?

Hsu H.J. · Bishop D.V.M.
University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
email Corresponding Author

Hsinjen Julie Hsu, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road

Oxford OX1 3UD (UK)

Tel. +44 1865 271 334, Fax +44 1865 281 255

E-Mail julie.hsu@psy.ox.ac.uk


References

  1. Albright, A., & Hayes, B. (2003). Rules versus analogies in the English past tenses: A computational/empirical study. Cognition, 90, 119–161.
  2. Billman, D. (1989). Systems of correlations in rule and category learning: Use of structured input in learning syntactic categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 127–155.
  3. Bishop, D.V.M. (1982). Comprehension of spoken, written, and signed sentences in childhood language disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 23, 1–20.
  4. Bishop, D.V.M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.
  5. Bishop, D.V.M. (2006). Developmental cognitive genetics: How psychology can inform genetics and vice versa. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1153–1168.

    External Resources

  6. Bishop, D.V.M., Adams, C.V., & Rosen, S. (2006). Resistance of grammatical impairment to computerized comprehension training in children with specific and non-specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 19–40.
  7. Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58.
  8. Clahsen, H. (1991). Child language and developmental dysphasia: Linguistic studies in the acquisition of German. Amsterdam: Benjamins Publishing Co.
  9. Coady, J.A., Evans, J.L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 1–40.
  10. Corkin, S. (1968). Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision. Neuropsychologia, 6, 255–265.

    External Resources

  11. Curran, T. (1997). Higher-order associative learning in amnesia: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 522–533.

    External Resources

  12. Edelman, S., & Waterfall, H. (2007). Behavioral and computational aspects of language and its acquisition. Physics of Life Reviews, 4, 253–277.

    External Resources

  13. Ellis, N.C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing – A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143–188.
  14. Gerken, L., Wilson, R., Lewis, W. (2005). Infants can use distributional cues to form syntactic categories. Journal of Child Language, 32, 249–268.
  15. Goad, H., & Rebellati, C. (1994). Pluralization in familial language impairment. In J. Matthews (Ed.), Linguistic aspects of familial language impairment. Special issue of the McGill Working Papers in Linguistics. Vol. 10 (pp. 24–40). Montreal: Department of Linguistics, McGill University.
  16. Gómez, R.L. (2002). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13, 431–436.
  17. Gómez, R.L., & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109–135.
  18. Gómez, R.L., & Lakusta, L. (2004). A first step in form-based category abstraction by 12-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 7, 567–580.
  19. Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50.
  20. Grunow, H., Spaulding, T.J., Gómez, R.L., & Plante, E. (2006). The effects of variation on learning word order rules by adults with and without language-based learning disabilities. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39, 158–170.
  21. Hsu, H., Tomblin, J.B., & Christiansen, M.H. (2008). The effect of variability in learning nonadjacent dependencies in typically-developing individuals and individuals with language impairments. In A. Owen (Chair), The role of input variability on language acquisition and use. Symposium presented at the XI International Congress for the Study of Child Language (IASCL), Edinburgh.
  22. Jiménez, L. (2008). Taking patterns for chunks: Is there any evidence of chunk learning in continuous serial reaction-time tasks? Psychological Research, 72, 387–396.
  23. Jones, M., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1997). A comparison of verb use in children with specific language impairment and their younger siblings. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1298–1313.
  24. Knowlton, B.J., & Squire, L.R. (1996). Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 169–181.
  25. Leonard, L. (1989). Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179–202.
  26. Liberman, M.D., Chang, G.Y., Chiao, J., Bookheimer, S.Y., & Knowlton, B.J. (2004). An event-related fMRI study of artificial grammar learning in a balanced chunk strength design. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 427–438.
  27. Lin, Y.A. (2006). Against the deficit in computational grammatical complexity hypothesis: A corpus-based study. Concentric: Studies in Linguistics, 32, 59–70.
  28. Lum, J.A.G., Gelgec, C., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2009). Procedural and declarative memory in children with and without specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 44, 1–19.
  29. Marchman, V.A., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development – A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21, 339–366.
  30. McNeill, D. (1966). Developmental psycholinguistics. In F. Smith & G. Miller (Eds.), The genesis of language (pp. 15–84). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  31. Norbury, C.F., Bishop, D.V.M., & Briscoe, J. (2001). Production of English finite verb morphology: A comparison of SLI and mild-moderate hearing impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44, 165–178.
  32. Onnis, L., Christiansen, M.H., Chater, N., & Gomez, R. (2003). Reduction of uncertainty in human sequential learning: Evidence from artificial grammar learning. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 886–891). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
  33. Onnis, L., Monaghan, P., Christiansen, M.H., & Chater, N. (2004). Variability is the spice of learning, and a crucial ingredient for detecting and generalizing in nonadjacent dependencies. Proceedings of the 26th Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah.
  34. Perruchet, P., & Pacton, S. (2006). Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 233–238.
  35. Pinker, S., & Ullman, M.T. (2002). The past and future of past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456–463.
  36. Plante, E., Gómez, R.L., & Gerken, L. (2002). Sensitivity to word order cues by normal and language/learning disabled adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 453–462.
  37. Poldrack, R.A., Clark, J., Paré-Blagoev, E.J., Shohamy, D., Creso Moyano, J., Myers, C., & Gluck, M.A. (2001). Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature, 414, 546–550.
  38. Rauch, S.L., Savage, C.R., Brown, H.D., Curran, T., Alpert, N.M., Kendrick, A., Fischman, A.J., & Kosslyn, S.M. (1995). A PET investigation of implicit and explicit sequence learning. Human Brain Mapping, 3, 271–286.

    External Resources

  39. Reber, P.J., & Squire, L.R. (1999). Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson’s disease. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 235–242.
  40. Redington, M., Chater, N., & Finch, S. (1998). Distributional information: A powerful cue for acquiring syntactic categories. Cognitive Science, 22, 425–469.

    External Resources

  41. Rice, M.L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P.L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863.
  42. Saffran, J.R. (2001). The use of predictive dependencies in language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 493–515.
  43. Samuelson, L.K. (2002). Statistical regularities in vocabulary guide language acquisition in connectionist models and 15–20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 38, 1016–1037.
  44. Schendan, H.E., Searl, M.M., Melrose, R.J., & Stern, C.E. (2003). An fMRI study of the role of the medial temporal lobe in implicit and explicit sequence learning. Neuron, 37, 1013–1025.
  45. Serratrice, L., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Joseph, K. (2003). The acquisition of past tense in pre-school children with specific language impairment and unaffected controls: regular and irregular forms. Linguistics, 41, 321–349.
  46. Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  47. Skipp, A., Windfuhr, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2002). Children’s grammatical categories of verb and noun: A comparative look at children with specific language impairment (SLI) and normal language (NL). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 253–271.
  48. Smith, L., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106, 1558–1568.
  49. Squire, L.R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 171–177.
  50. Stokes, S.F., & Fletcher, P. (2000). Lexical diversity and productivity in Cantonese-speaking children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 527–541.
  51. Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 156–163.
  52. Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  53. Tomblin, J.B., Mainela-Arnold, E., Zhang, X. (2007). Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Language Learning and Development, 3, 269–293.
  54. Ullman, M.T. (2001). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 37–69.
  55. Ullman, M.T., & Pierpont, E.I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399–433.
  56. van der Lely, H.K.J. (1997). Language and cognitive development in a grammatical SLI boy: Modularity and innateness. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 10, 75–107.
  57. van der Lely, H.K.J., & Ullman, M. (2001). Past tense morphology in specific language impaired and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 177–217.
  58. Wexler, K. (1994). Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations. In D. Lightfoot & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Verb movement (pp. 305–350). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  59. Witt, K., Nühsma, A., & Deuschl, G. (2002). Intact artificial grammar learning in patients with cerebellar degeneration and advanced Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1534–1540.